DOI: 10.17586/1023-5086-2019-86-11-14-20
УДК: 612.843
High-frequency filters in ontogenesis
Full text «Opticheskii Zhurnal»
Full text on elibrary.ru
Publication in Journal of Optical Technology
Бондарко В.М. Высокочастотные фильтры в онтогенезе // Оптический журнал. 2019. Т. 86. № 11. С. 14–20. http://doi.org/10.17586/1023-5086-2019-86-11-14-20
Bondarko V.M. High-frequency filters in ontogenesis [in Russian] // Opticheskii Zhurnal. 2019. V. 86. № 11. P. 14–20. http://doi.org/10.17586/1023-5086-2019-86-11-14-20
V. M. Bondarko, "High-frequency filters in ontogenesis," Journal of Optical Technology. 86(11), 686-690 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1364/JOT.86.000686
We measure the minimum dimensions of rectangular gratings and Landolt Cs at which it became possible for observers to determine orientation. The experiment used 677 observers ranging from 4 to 22 years of age. A parallel study was also performed to investigate the effect of background images on recognition of these same stimuli (the Landolt Cs were surrounded by a striped pattern, and the gratings were surrounded by a background of other gratings). The resulting minimum dimensions for each stimulus steadily decreased, reaching adult human recognition levels as the observer approached 11 years of age. All observers noted degradation of stimulus recognition when the background images were in close proximity (the crowding effect). The stimulus-background separation at which stimulus detection began to become degraded decreased until age 12 for Landolt Cs and until age 16 for gratings. The data are discussed for the purposes of considering revisions to high-frequency filter management in ontogenesis. The filters become narrow band and tuned to a higher spatial frequency with increasing observer age.
ontogenesis, rectangular gratings, Landolt Cs, crowding effect, high-frequency filters
Acknowledgements:The research was supported by the Program for Fundamental Research at Government-Affiliated Academic Institutions for 2013-2020 (GP-14, section 63).
OCIS codes: 330.7326, 330.4060, 330.5510
References:1. V. Lutsiv, “Convolutional deep-learning artificial neural networks,” J. Opt. Technol. 82(8), 499–508 (2015) [Opt. Zh. 82(8), 11–23 (2015)].
2. Yu. E. Shelepin, V. M. Bondarko, and M. V. Danilova, “Foveola structure and model for pyramidal structure of visual system,” Sens. Sist. 9(1), 87–97 (1995).
3. M. C. Flom, F. W. Weymouth, and D. Kahneman, “Visual resolution and contour interaction,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. 53, 1026–1032 (1963).
4. J. Atkinson, “Review of human visual development: crowding and dyslexia,” in Vision and Visual Dyslexia, J. F. Stein, ed. (1991), vol. 13, pp. 44–57.
5. V. M. Bondarko and M. V. Danilova, “Relationship between crowding effect and operation of high-frequency spatial elements,” Sens. Sist. 16(2), 89–99 (2002).
6. M. V. Danilova and V. M. Bondarko, “Foveal contour interactions and crowding effects at the resolution limit of the visual system,” J. Vision 7(25), 1–18 (2007).
7. H. R. Wilson and D. J. Gelb, “Modified line-element theory for spatial-frequency and width discrimination,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1, 124–131 (1984).
8. Y. E. Shelepin and V. M. Bondarko, “Resolving ability and image discretization in the visual system,” Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 34(2), 147–157 (2004).
9. L. A. Semenov, N. D. Chernova, and V. M. Bondarko, “Measurement of visual acuity and crowding effect in 3–9-year-old children,” Hum. Physiol. 26, 16–20 (2000) [Fiziol. Chel. 26, 67–72 (2000)].
10. L. A. Semenov, N. D. Chernova, and V. M. Bondarko, “Age-related dynamics of discrimination of the orientation of rectangular gratings at the resolution limit of the visual system,” Hum. Physiol. 28, 383–390 (2002) [Fiziol. Chel. 28(4), 5–12 (2002)].
11. V. M. Bondarko and L. A. Semenov, “Visual acuity and the crowding effect in 8- to 17-year-old schoolchildren,” Hum. Physiol. 31, 532–538 (2005) [Fiziol. Chel. 31(5), 13–21 (2005)].
12. G. B. Wetherill and H. Levitt, “Sequential estimation of points on a psychometric function,” Br. J. Math. Stat. Psychol. 18, 1–10 (1965).
13. N. A. Kaliteevsky, V. E. Semenov, V. D. Glezer, and V. E. Gauselman, “Algorithm of invariant image description by the use of a modified Gabor transform,” Appl. Opt. 33, 5256–5261 (1994).
14. V. D. Glezer, Vision and Mind (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, 1995; Nauka, Leningrad, 1985).
15. C. Blakemore and F. W. Campbell, “On the existence of neurones in the human visual system selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal images,” J. Physiol. 203, 237–260 (1969).
16. F. W. Campbell and J. G. Robson, “Application of Fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings,” J. Physiol. 197, 551–566 (1968).
17. H. R. Wilson, “Development of spatiotemporal mechanisms in infant vision,” Vis. Res. 28, 611–628 (1988).
18. U. Polat and D. Sagi, “Lateral interactions between spatial channels: suppression and facilitation revealed by lateral masking experiments,” Vis. Res. 33, 993–999 (1993).
19. S. A. Koskin, É. V. Bo˘ıko, and Yu. E. Shelepin, “Modern methods of measuring the resolving power of the visual system,” J. Opt. Technol. 75(1), 17–20 (2008) [Opt. Zh. 75(1), 22–27 (2008)].
20. V. M. Bondarko and M. V. Danilova, “What spatial frequency do we use to detect the orientation of a Landolt C?” Vis. Res. 37, 2153–2156 (1997).
21. V. M. Bondarko and L. A. Semenov, “Orientation selectivity and visual acuity in schoolchildren and adults,” Hum. Physiol. 43, 259–264 (2017) [Fiziol. Chel. 43(2), 1–7 (2017)].